Life satisfaction and social desirability across the late life span: What relationship?

2014 ◽  
Vol 24 (1) ◽  
pp. 241-244 ◽  
Author(s):  
Maria Chiara Fastame ◽  
Maria Pietronilla Penna ◽  
Paul Kenneth Hitchcott
2019 ◽  
Vol 32 (3) ◽  
pp. 515-533 ◽  
Author(s):  
Maria Chiara Fastame ◽  
Maria Pietronilla Penna ◽  
Paul Kenneth Hitchcott

2006 ◽  
Author(s):  
Brendan M. Baird ◽  
Richard E. Lucas ◽  
M. Brent Donnellan
Keyword(s):  

1983 ◽  
Vol 23 (3) ◽  
pp. 261-265 ◽  
Author(s):  
P. A. Baur ◽  
M. A. Okun
Keyword(s):  

2020 ◽  
Vol 21 (1) ◽  
pp. 6-41 ◽  
Author(s):  
Martin Lövdén ◽  
Laura Fratiglioni ◽  
M. Maria Glymour ◽  
Ulman Lindenberger ◽  
Elliot M. Tucker-Drob

Cognitive abilities are important predictors of educational and occupational performance, socioeconomic attainment, health, and longevity. Declines in cognitive abilities are linked to impairments in older adults’ everyday functions, but people differ from one another in their rates of cognitive decline over the course of adulthood and old age. Hence, identifying factors that protect against compromised late-life cognition is of great societal interest. The number of years of formal education completed by individuals is positively correlated with their cognitive function throughout adulthood and predicts lower risk of dementia late in life. These observations have led to the propositions that prolonging education might (a) affect cognitive ability and (b) attenuate aging-associated declines in cognition. We evaluate these propositions by reviewing the literature on educational attainment and cognitive aging, including recent analyses of data harmonized across multiple longitudinal cohort studies and related meta-analyses. In line with the first proposition, the evidence indicates that educational attainment has positive effects on cognitive function. We also find evidence that cognitive abilities are associated with selection into longer durations of education and that there are common factors (e.g., parental socioeconomic resources) that affect both educational attainment and cognitive development. There is likely reciprocal interplay among these factors, and among cognitive abilities, during development. Education–cognitive ability associations are apparent across the entire adult life span and across the full range of education levels, including (to some degree) tertiary education. However, contrary to the second proposition, we find that associations between education and aging-associated cognitive declines are negligible and that a threshold model of dementia can account for the association between educational attainment and late-life dementia risk. We conclude that educational attainment exerts its influences on late-life cognitive function primarily by contributing to individual differences in cognitive skills that emerge in early adulthood but persist into older age. We also note that the widespread absence of educational influences on rates of cognitive decline puts constraints on theoretical notions of cognitive aging, such as the concepts of cognitive reserve and brain maintenance. Improving the conditions that shape development during the first decades of life carries great potential for improving cognitive ability in early adulthood and for reducing public-health burdens related to cognitive aging and dementia.


2005 ◽  
Vol 62 (5) ◽  
pp. 565 ◽  
Author(s):  
Jeremy M. Silverman ◽  
Gregory Ciresi ◽  
Christopher J. Smith ◽  
Deborah B. Marin ◽  
Michal Schnaider-Beeri

Author(s):  
Arthur S. Kraus

AbstractAn assumed fixed maximum life span was involved in a much publicized prediction of a compression of morbidity in late life. The maximum life span has major weaknesses as a measure. The “usual life span“, or age to which 1.0% of a birth cohort survives, is suggested as a more useful measure.Statistics were derived from recent United States and Canadian censuses on the age above which 1.0% of the population is contained, from recent United States and Canadian mortality data on the age above which 1.0% of the deaths were reported, and from recent Canadian life tables on the age to which 1.0% of a cohort would survive. These statistics suggest that the usual life span increased at least 2 years in the 1960–1980 period in North America.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document