scholarly journals Bird Usage of Black Marasmius Fibers as Nest Material

Author(s):  
Haris Rana ◽  
Shayla Smithson ◽  
Jack Jackson ◽  
Ragupathy Kannan
Keyword(s):  
Author(s):  
Vivien Cosandey ◽  
Robin Séchaud ◽  
Paul Béziers ◽  
Yannick Chittaro ◽  
Andreas Sanchez ◽  
...  

AbstractBird nests are specialized habitats because of their particular composition including nest detritus and bird droppings. In consequence, they attract a specialized arthropod community considered as nidicolous, which includes species only found in bird nests (strictly nidicolous) or sometimes found in bird nests (facultatively nidicolous). Because the factors influencing the entomofauna in bird nests are poorly understood, in autumn 2019, we collected nest material in 86 Barn Owl (Tyto alba) nest boxes. We investigated whether the invertebrate species richness was related to Barn Owl nest box occupancy, the density of available nest boxes and the landscape structure. We found 3,321 nidicolous beetle specimens belonging to 24 species. Species richness of strictly nidicolous beetles was 2.7 times higher in nest boxes occupied by a family of Barn Owls the previous spring compared to unoccupied nest boxes. It was also higher in sites that were more often occupied by Barn Owls in the five previous years and in areas surrounded by a higher proportion of crop fields. For facultatively nidicolous beetles, the density of Barn Owl nest boxes enhanced the species richness. In conclusion, our study suggests that the strictly nidicolous beetles benefit from occupied nest boxes of Barn Owls, whereas facultatively nidicolous beetles look for nest boxes independently of whether Barn Owls occupy them. Our study highlights the importance of bird nests for a suite of invertebrates.


Author(s):  
Heung-Chul Kim ◽  
Ju Jiang ◽  
Jun Hang ◽  
Su Yeon Kim ◽  
Seok-Min Yun ◽  
...  

Abstract In a follow-up to the investigations of soft ticks identified from seabird nest soil and litter collected from coastal islands of the Republic of Korea (ROK), Ornithodoros sawaii and Ornithodoros capensis were assessed for the presence and identification of rickettsiae. Ticks collected from samples of 50–100 g of nest litter and soil from seabird nests were identified individually by morphological techniques, and species confirmed by sequencing of the mt-rrs gene. Subsequently, tick DNA preparations were screened for the presence of rickettsiae using a genus-specific nested PCR (nPCR) assay targeting the 17 kDa antigen gene. The amplicons from the 17 kDa assay and two additional nPCR assays targeting the gltA and ompB gene fragments were sequenced and used to identify the rickettsiae. A total of 134 soft ticks belonging to two species, O. sawaii Kitaoka & Suzuki 1973 (n = 125) and O. capensis Neumann 1901 (n = 9), were collected. Rickettsia lusitaniae DNA was detected and identified among O. sawaii ticks (n = 11, 8.8%) collected from nest litter and soil of the Japanese murrelet (Synthliboramphus wumizusume Temminck 1836) at Gugul Island along the western coastal area of the ROK. This study confirmed for the first time the presence of R. lusitaniae associated with O. sawaii collected from migratory seabird nests in the ROK.


Parasitology ◽  
1962 ◽  
Vol 52 (1-2) ◽  
pp. 113-132 ◽  
Author(s):  
John Keith Bates ◽  
Miriam Rothschild

1. Factors controlling the distribution of the bird fleas Ceratophyllus styx, the sand-martin flea, C. gallinae, the hen flea, and Dasypsylla gallinulae, the blackbird flea, were investigated in the field.2. Sand-martin fleas (C. styx) pass the winter as adults within cocoons in the old nests of their host. Observations indicate that the fleas are stimulated to emerge from the cocoons by the rise in temperature in the spring, and some of the adult fleas emigrate in the spring and invade new nest burrows. Observations and experiments showed that sand-martin fleas disperse from old burrows both laterally and vertically and that emigrating fleas could reach areas as far as 33·8 m. from the old nests. The detection of the new burrow entrances is not due to vision, or to the recognition of difference of humidity between the burrow and the cliff face, or to the reaction to air current differences between the burrow and the cliff face, but to recognition of the horizontal floor of the burrows and a tendency to congregate upon horizontal surfaces. How the fleas distinguish the horizontal burrow surface from the cliff top is still unknown. The colonization of new burrows by the fleas does not occur at night. It is suggested that, in addition to finding their hosts in the spring by invading new burrows, sand-martin fleas may jump on to the birds when they are hovering near the cliff face. Many adult fleas leave the burrows within 3 days of the fledging time of the young sand-martins but a small number remain within the nest. The fate of these specimens is not known.3. C. gallinae, the hen flea, and D. gallinulae, the blackbird flea, pass the winter principally as adults enclosed in the cocoons in the old nest material. In the spring they emerge from the cocoons, emigrate away from the old nest and are free-living on the ground. Probably the adult C. gallinae and D. gallinulae jump on to birds when they are feeding on the ground in the spring.* Therefore the absence of hen fleas in nests situated on the ground or in open nests built in low vegetation, is not due to lack of opportunity of contact between the adult flea and the birds which construct such nests.4. It is concluded that in host-seeking, adults of C. gallinae and D. gallinulae emigrate away from old nests in the spring and come in contact with the host birds on the ground in the birds' feeding area, whereas C. styx adults emigrate into the hosts' breeding area and come in contact with the birds in the new nesting burrows.


Behaviour sequences commonly consist of highly variable appetitive phases leading to rather fixed consummatory acts. Action-pattern rigidity is typical of the terminal moments of a reaction chain. This basic fact is all too often obscured by the artificial conditions of behaviour studies. Observations on laboratory or captive animals tend to conceal the degree of variability of the earlier phases of each sequence. The simplicity and sterility of the unnatural environment offered to the animal causes differential damage to its motoric performance, attacking the early stages more and the later stages less. A caged animal will feed, drink, nest and copulate, but it cannot set off on lengthy quests for food, water, nest material or a mate. Notorious laboratory devices such as the Skinner-box have served to eliminate totally any possibility for motoric variability. The emphasis in laboratory studies of this kind has been steadfastly concentrated on the variability in the relationship between simple stimuli and an artificially rigidified response. Although the study of this (SR) relationship is an important aspect of animal psychology, it is extremely misleading to overstress its importance as has been done so often in the past. To equate it with the whole topic of animal behaviour is like claiming that the gaming rooms of Las Vegas reflect the whole of human endeavour.


Behaviour ◽  
1969 ◽  
Vol 34 (1-2) ◽  
pp. 1-16 ◽  
Author(s):  
Victor H. Denenberg ◽  
M.X. Zarrow ◽  
Rhoda E. Taylor

AbstractA technique has been developed to quantify nest building in rats. Small wooden dowels are provided as nest material. Rats shred the dowels, and the degree of nest building is determined by the amount shredded daily. Pregnant females show a marked increase in dowel shredding at or just prior to the time of parturition; shredding falls precipitously after parturition. Males and nonpregnant females show no such pattern over an equivalent period of time; their dowel shredding, in fact, decreases over time. A series of experiments were carried out involving hormone manipulations of pregnant and nonpregnant females. The only significant finding was that progesterone reduced the percentage of females which shredded dowels and also delayed the time of onset of this behavior. When nonpregnant females and males were exposed to cool ambient temperatures, dowel shredding increased markedly. On the other hand, exposing females to a warm temperature blocked dowel shredding behavior. Some similarities and differences between these findings and findings for the rabbit and mouse are discussed.


2015 ◽  
Vol 38 (0) ◽  
pp. 9 ◽  
Author(s):  
Arild Breistøl ◽  
Göran Högstedt ◽  
Terje Lislevand

Behaviour ◽  
2021 ◽  
pp. 1-13
Author(s):  
Auke-Florian Hiemstra ◽  
Barbara Gravendeel ◽  
Menno Schilthuizen

Abstract Here we present the first cases of birds using artificial plants as nest material. We report our findings for the common coot (Fulica atra) from Leiden, the Netherlands, in 2019. This is the first population of freshwater birds studied for its use of anthropogenic nest materials, and together with another report from the same year, the earliest case of an entire bird population with plastic in all nests. We also report the first artificial plants used as nesting material by birds, and discuss the implications of their usage as such.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Shenghui Wang ◽  
Ximing Wang ◽  
Fengtian Guo ◽  
Bing Han ◽  
Xiaoyu Wang

1983 ◽  
Vol 38 (5-6) ◽  
pp. 508-510 ◽  
Author(s):  
K. Horstmann

The interior of wood ant nests (Hymenoptera, Formicidae, Formica polyctena Förster) and artificial piles of nest material has been heated with heating wires in the field. In the artificial piles a heating power of 1 - 5 Watt is suffi­cient to cause a temperature increase equivalent to that caused by ants in their nests without additional heating. Whereas in artificial piles temperatures increase with in­creasing heating power, the ants prevent temperatures to exceed 34.6 degrees C in their natural nests, even if these are heated with a power of more than 20 Watt.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document