reversed order
Recently Published Documents


TOTAL DOCUMENTS

65
(FIVE YEARS 17)

H-INDEX

11
(FIVE YEARS 1)

2021 ◽  
Vol 20 ◽  
pp. 606-613
Author(s):  
Farrukh Jamal ◽  
Christophe Chesneau

The power Ailamujia distribution has been successfully developed in statistics, both theoretically and practically, performing well in the fitting of various types of data. This paper investigates the moment properties of the associated order, reversed order and upper record statistics, which are indeed unexplored aspects of this distribution. In particular, the exact expressions for the single moments of the order and reversed order statistics are provided. Some recurrence relationships for both single and product moments for the order and upper record statistics are proved. For additional goals, certain joint distributions are also given.


Author(s):  
Hrach Martirosyan

There are a great number of Armenian compound personal names with the element šah ‘king’ of Iranian origin (Middle Persian and New Persian šāh ‘king’). It occurs: (1) in both masculine and feminine names; (2) with both native Armenian and foreign components; (3) either as the first or the second component; (4) often in doublet forms with a reversed order of the components. For instance: masculine Šah-amir and Amir-šah, Šah-paron and Paron-šah, Vahram-šah; feminine: Šah-xat‘un and Xat‘un-šah, Šah-tikin. Also note masc. Šah-aziz vs. fem. Aziz-šah, masc․ Sult‘an-šah vs. fem. Šah-sult‘an, masc. Melik‘-šah vs. fem. Šah-melē/ik‘ (the latter is sometimes masculine, cf. Middle Persian > Syriac Šāh-malīk, also masculine ). This paper aims to interpret two hapax legomena in which the component šah became synchronically unanalyzable due to phonological changes. In one of them, šah is the second member of the name (gen. Artamšin < *Artam-šah/y-in), whereas in the other it is the first one (Šaštʻi < *Šah-stʻi ‘Šah-Lady’).


2021 ◽  
pp. 088626052110300
Author(s):  
Paulina Tomaszewska ◽  
Isabell Schuster ◽  
Juliette Marchewka ◽  
Barbara Krahé

Measures designed to collect prevalence reports of sexual victimization need to be robust against variations of question context to yield valid findings. Previous research has examined variations in the order in which questions about unwanted sexual acts and questions about coercive tactics are presented. The current study examined potential effects of the order in which coercive tactics are presented on self-reported prevalence rates of sexual victimization. The following two versions of the Sexual Aggression and Victimization Scale (SAV-S), a validated measure for studying sexual aggression victimization and perpetration in college students, were used: (a) the standard version in which the physical-force items were presented first and the items referring to the use of verbal pressure were presented last, and (b) a reversed order in which the verbal-pressure items were presented first and the physical-force items were presented last. Items referring to the exploitation of the victim’s inability to resist were placed in the middle in both versions. In a sample of 856 participants from Germany (475 female, 381 male, mean age of 24 years), most of whom were university students, 80.4% of women and 55.3% of men reported at least one experience of sexual victimization since the age of 14. No order effects on overall victimization rates were found. For both genders, the victimization rate through verbal pressure was higher when this tactic was presented first. Victimization rates through the threat or use of force were higher in the force-first than in the force-last condition for women, but could not be tested for men due to small cell sizes. No order effects were found for both men and women on reports of victimization through exploiting the inability to resist. The implications of the results for the reliable measurement of sexual aggression are discussed.


Author(s):  
Hiroyuki Katsuto ◽  
Ryuichi Okamoto ◽  
Tomonari Sumi ◽  
Kenichiro Koga

2021 ◽  
Vol 1933 (1) ◽  
pp. 012075
Author(s):  
R A Apsari ◽  
S Sariyasa ◽  
J Junaidi ◽  
R Y Tyaningsih ◽  
G Gunawan
Keyword(s):  

2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Haocong Li ◽  
Yong Li ◽  
Wenfeng Wang ◽  
Qun Wan ◽  
Xiang-Yang Yu ◽  
...  

Abstract Triazole pesticide is a widely-used pesticide for the control of pathogenic fungi in crops and its extensive use has caused food safety issues due to its uptake byedible crops. The residue of triazole pesticides has caused many toxicity risks and food safety problems. In this study, the uptake, translocation, and subcellular distribution of three triazole pesticides (triadimefon, tebuconazole, and epoxiconazole) in rice were investigated. The results showed that the three selected triazole pesticides could be taken up by rice roots, but their distribution in plant tissues was different. The accumulation of the three pesticides in rice root followed the order of epoxiconazole > tebuconazole > triadimefon, while a reversed order was observed in rice shoots. The movement of triazole pesticides within rice tissues involved both symplast and apoplast pathways, with triadimefon preferentially through the symplast pathway and epoxiconazole throughby the symplast pathway. The subcellular distribution reveals that all pesticides have a higher proportion in cell walls than in cell organelles and soluble components. Epoxiconazole has the highest accumulated capacity in the cell wall (45%-67%) and triadimefon was more concentrated in the soluble components (24%-29%). However, there exists no significant difference in the amount of three pesticides in cell organelles. The pesticide uptake, the movement of pesticides in symplast and apoplast, and the subcellular distribution of the three pesticides are related to the hydrophobicity of the three pesticides.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
András Ecker ◽  
Bence Bagi ◽  
Eszter Vértes ◽  
Orsolya Steinbach-Németh ◽  
Mária Karlócai ◽  
...  

Abstract Hippocampal place cells are activated sequentially as an animal explores its environment. These activity sequences are internally recreated (``replayed'), either in the same or reversed order, during bursts of activity (sharp wave-ripples; SWRs) that occur in sleep and awake rest. SWR-associated replay is thought to be critical for the creation and maintenance of long-term memory. In order to identify the cellular and network mechanisms of SWRs and replay, we constructed and simulated a data-driven model of area CA3 of the hippocampus. Our results show that the chain-like structure of recurrent excitatory interactions established during learning not only determines the content of replay, but is essential for the generation of the SWRs as well. We find that bidirectional replay requires the interplay of the experimentally confirmed, temporally symmetric plasticity rule, and cellular adaptation. Our model provides a unifying framework for diverse phenomena involving hippocampal plasticity, representations, and dynamics, and suggests that the structured neural codes induced by learning may have greater influence over cortical network states than previously appreciated.


Author(s):  
Guro Granerud ◽  
Erik Arntzen

AbstractIn the present study, two typically developing 4-year-old children, Pete and Joe, were trained six conditional discriminations and tested for the formation of three 3-member equivalence classes. Pete and Joe did not establish the AC relation within 600 trials and were given two conditions of preliminary training, including naming of stimuli with two different stimulus sets. Pete started with preliminary training with common naming of stimuli, followed by conditional-discrimination training and testing for emergent relations, and continued with preliminary training on individual naming of stimuli, followed by the same training and testing as described previously. Joe experienced the same conditions but in reversed order. Pete responded in accordance with equivalence in the second round in the condition with common naming. In the first round of testing in the condition with individual naming, he responded in accordance with equivalence. In the condition with individual naming, Joe did not respond in accordance with stimulus equivalence but established all of the directly trained relations during training. In the condition with common naming, he responded in accordance with equivalence in the first round of testing. The results from the experiment support earlier findings that both common and individual naming could facilitate the emergence of equivalence classes.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Andras Ecker ◽  
Bence Bagi ◽  
Eszter Vertes ◽  
Orsolya Steinbach-Nemeth ◽  
Maria Rita Karlocai ◽  
...  

Hippocampal place cells are activated sequentially as an animal explores its environment. These activity sequences are internally recreated ("replayed"), either in the same or reversed order, during bursts of activity (sharp wave-ripples; SWRs) that occur in sleep and awake rest. SWR-associated replay is thought to be critical for the creation and maintenance of long-term memory. We sought to identify the cellular and network mechanisms of SWRs and replay by constructing and simulating a data-driven model of area CA3 of the hippocampus. Our results show that the structure of recurrent excitatory interactions established during learning not only determines the content of replay, but is essential for the generation of the SWRs as well. We find that bidirectional replay requires the interplay of the experimentally confirmed, temporally symmetric plasticity rule, and cellular adaptation. Our model provides a unifying framework for diverse phenomena involving hippocampal plasticity, representations, and dynamics.


Author(s):  
Claudia Braun ◽  
Sebastian Fischer ◽  
Nils Eckardt

AbstractInterpreting other’s actions is a very important ability not only in social life, but also in interactive sports. Previous experiments have demonstrated good estimation performances for the weight of lifted objects through point-light displays. The basis for these performances is commonly assigned to the concept of motor simulation regarding observed actions. In this study, we investigated the weak version of the motor simulation hypothesis which claims that the goal of an observed action strongly influences its understanding (Fogassi, Ferrari, Gesierich, Rozzi, Chersi, & Rizzolatti, 2005). Therefore, we conducted a weight judgement task with point-light displays and showed participants videos of a model lifting and lowering three different weights. The experimental manipulation consisted of a goal change of these actions by showing the videos normal and in a time-reversed order of sequence. The results show a systematic overestimation of weights for time-reversed lowering actions (thus looking like lifting actions) while weight estimations for time-reversed lifting actions did not differ from the original playback direction. The results are discussed in terms of motor simulation and different kinematic profiles of the presented actions.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document